1

Christine Lehnen

Exploring Narratives’ Powers of Emotional Persuasion through Character Involvement: A Working Heuristic

(Abstract)

Full-length article in: JLT 10/2 (2016), 247–270.

Narratives change people’s minds. This has been shown as early as forty-five years ago, but recently, the persuasive and emotional effects of narratives have received fresh attention, as well as emotions themselves. Both psychology and literature researchers have begun to investigate the particularities of the emotional functioning of narratives and how they persuade, i. e. influence attitudes through having readers experience emotions. Their research, combined with recent appraisal theories of vicarious (i. e. other-oriented) emotions, indicates that the emotional effects of a narrative can be hypothesized about by critics through analyzing the text itself, i. e. the textual situations that characters are placed in and how the contextualized readers may simulate and appraise these situations. These findings, however, have not yet been turned into a workable heuristic which would allow literary critics to analyse textual narratives for their emotional and persuasive effect. I set out to do so in this paper.

First, I summarize recent research conducted on and theories proposed for the emotional effect of character involvement in narratives. Especially noteworthy are Howard Sklar’s The Art of Sympathy in Fiction and Claudia Hillebrandt’s and Elisabeth Kampmann’s volume Sympathie und Literatur, as well as Suzanne Keen’s slightly older Empathy and the Novel. Hillebrandt’s and Kampmann’s definition of sympathy – feeling for someone when readers’ and texts’ value horizons match – informs the majority of contributions to the volume. While impressively illuminating the internal value horizons of texts as envisioned at their point of creation, this approach neither allows for nor aims at hypotheses on widespread emotional reader response or persuasion.

Sklar’s approach incorporates an everyday understanding of ›sympathy‹, i. e. of feeling sorry or pity for someone. His study – forming a hypothesis through analysis, testing it empirically, and ensuring inter-coder-variability – is to be commended, I would argue, however, that sympathy covers only a small amount of the possible vicarious emotions a reader may feel while experiencing a narrative.

Keen, the only one to use empathy, not sympathy, in her title, doubts that reading narratives (in particular high-brow literature) directly leads to acts of altruism. In her study, she does not primarily develop a heuristic for hypothesizing on empirical reader response, but deconstructs researchers’, readers’ and writers’ attitudes concerning narrative and empathy, and consequently proposes a heuristic for analyzing authorial strategies of empathy, which does not seem suitable for predicting contextualized reader response.

Thus I turn to empirical research on narrative emotional persuasion (Ch. 2). Scholars concerned have found that narratives are more likely than expository texts to lead to attitude changes, and they have linked the degree (quantity) of attitude change to the degree of transportation experienced by readers. Intending to expand their findings to include the quality of attitude change, I combine findings from appraisal theory of vicarious emotions with those of simulation theory, developing emotions of narrative response (Ch. 3). These emotions, like other vicarious emotions, result from readers appraising situations that persons or characters are in. Situations are appraised along six appraisal dimensions: human/situational control, self/other responsibility, certain/uncertain, low/high attention, pleasantness/unpleasantness, low/high effort. Certain combinations and intensity of appraisal dimensions (›appraisal patterns‹) predict certain ›emotions‹, although emotions are not to be understood as coherent units or Platonic types, but as the unbounded grouping of affective and cognitive processes under socially constructed labels. The conditions for appraisal in texts are information sufficiency and valence.

I propose to operationalize the text-situated character-focused conditions of appraisal and the process of appraisal itself as follows: (i) identify passages in the narrative that portray characters in situations pertinent to the general goalof avoiding unpleasantness/seeking out pleasantness – ›sympathy‹ (general valence); (ii) identify passages that portray characters in situations pertinent to specific goals– ›empathy‹ (specific valence); (iii) identify passages where inferencesabout a character may be drawn – ›identification‹ (information sufficiency). Once these text passages or textual situations are identified, hypotheses on the likely emotional judgements along the appraisal dimensions are formed by analysing the (iv) conflicts into which characters enter, their outcome, and what has caused that particular outcome. It is here, I propose, that emotional attitude change or the emotional persuasiveness of narratives enters the picture through making characters (and readers) suffer – or not (Ch. 4).

I then sketch an analysis of Suzanne Collins’ immensely successful ›dystopian‹ novel The Hunger Games, finding that, while emotional involvement is likely, the textual situations the novel presents at the end are likely to be appraised as positive and self-directed (resulting in pride) (Ch. 5). An empirical test using online reviews shows that the hypotheses’ predictions turn out to be accurate (Ch. 6): Emotional persuasion, operationalized in this specific case as delivering a warning, is reported only by a tenth of readers reporting emotional reaction, whereas 75 % of them report successful character involvement. This indicates that the heuristic produces hypotheses with predictive power, allowing for greater differentiation for questions of qualitative reader persuasion. Research on the emotional persuasion of narratives is still in its infancy, but the heuristic developed here may provide a stepping stone towards developing and working on a greater range of questions with more predictive power.

Narratives change people’s minds. This has been shown as early as forty-five years ago, but recently, the persuasive and emotional effects of narratives have received fresh attention, as well as emotions themselves. Both psychology and literature researchers have begun to investigate the particularities of the emotional functioning of narratives and how they persuade, i. e. influence attitudes through having readers experience emotions. Their research, combined with recent appraisal theories of vicarious (i. e. other-oriented) emotions, indicates that the emotional effects of a narrative can be hypothesized about by critics through analyzing the text itself, i. e. the textual situations that characters are placed in and how the contextualized readers may simulate and appraise these situations. These findings, however, have not yet been turned into a workable heuristic which would allow literary critics to analyse textual narratives for their emotional and persuasive effect. I set out to do so in this paper.

First, I summarize recent research conducted on and theories proposed for the emotional effect of character involvement in narratives. Especially noteworthy are Howard Sklar’s The Art of Sympathy in Fiction and Claudia Hillebrandt’s and Elisabeth Kampmann’s volume Sympathie und Literatur, as well as Suzanne Keen’s slightly older Empathy and the Novel. Hillebrandt’s and Kampmann’s definition of sympathy – feeling for someone when readers’ and texts’ value horizons match – informs the majority of contributions to the volume. While impressively illu minating the internal value horizons of texts as envisioned at their point of creation, this approach neither allows for nor aims at hypotheses on widespread emotional reader response or persuasion.

Sklar’s approach incorporates an everyday understanding of ›sympathy‹, i. e. of feeling sorry or pity for someone. His study – forming a hypothesis through analysis, testing it empirically, and ensuring inter-coder-variability – is to be commended, I would argue, however, that sympathy covers only a small amount of the possible vicarious emotions a reader may feel while experiencing a narrative.

Keen, the only one to use empathy, not sympathy, in her title, doubts that reading narratives (in particular high-brow literature) directly leads to acts of altruism. In her study, she does not primarily develop a heuristic for hypothesizing on empirical reader response, but deconstructs researchers’, readers’ and writers’ attitudes concerning narrative and empathy, and consequently proposes a heuristic for analyzing authorial strategies of empathy, which does not seem suitable for predicting contextualized reader response.

Thus I turn to empirical research on narrative emotional persuasion (Ch. 2). Scholars concerned have found that narratives are more likely than expository texts to lead to attitude changes, and they have linked the degree (quantity) of attitude change to the degree of transportation experienced by readers. Intending to expand their findings to include the quality of attitude change, I combine findings from appraisal theory of vicarious emotions with those of simulation theory, developing emotions of narrative response (Ch. 3). These emotions, like other vicarious emotions, result from readers appraising situations that persons or characters are in. Situations are appraised along six appraisal dimensions: human/situational control, self/other responsibility, certain/uncertain, low/high attention, pleasantness/unpleasantness, low/high effort. Certain combinations and intensity of appraisal dimensions (›appraisal patterns‹) predict certain ›emotions‹, although emotions are not to be understood as coherent units or Platonic types, but as the unbounded grouping of affective and cognitive processes under socially constructed labels. The conditions for appraisal in texts are information sufficiency and valence.

I propose to operationalize the text-situated character-focused conditions of appraisal and the process of appraisal itself as follows: (i) identify passages in the narrative that portray characters in situations pertinent to the general goalof avoiding unpleasantness/seeking out pleasantness – ›sympathy‹ (general valence); (ii) identify passages that portray characters in situations pertinent to specific goals– ›empathy‹ (specific valence); (iii) identify passages where inferencesabout a character may be drawn – ›identification‹ (information sufficiency). Once these text passages or textual situations are identified, hypotheses on the likely emotional judgements along the appraisal dimensions are formed by analysing the (iv) conflicts into which characters enter, their outcome, and what has caused that particular outcome. It is here, I propose, that emotional attitude change or the emotional persuasiveness of narratives enters the picture through making characters (and readers) suffer – or not (Ch. 4).

I then sketch an analysis of Suzanne Collins’ immensely successful ›dystopian‹ novel The Hunger Games, finding that, while emotional involvement is likely, the textual situations the novel presents at the end are likely to be appraised as positive and self-directed (resulting in pride) (Ch. 5). An empirical test using online reviews shows that the hypotheses’ predictions turn out to be accurate (Ch. 6): Emotional persuasion, operationalized in this specific case as delivering a warning, is reported only by a tenth of readers reporting emotional reaction, whereas 75 % of them report successful character involvement. This indicates that the heuristic produces hypotheses with predictive power, allowing for greater differentiation for questions of qualitative reader persuasion. Research on the emotional persuasion of narratives is still in its infancy, but the heuristic developed here may provide a stepping stone towards developing and working on a greater range of questions with more predictive power.

References

Ames, Daniel R./Gita V. Johar, I’ll Know What You’re Like When I See How You Feel. How and When Affective Displays Influence Behavior-Based Impressions, Psychological Science 20:5 (2009), 586–593. [Crossref] [Web of Science]

Anz, Thomas, Regeln der Sympathielenkung. Normative und deskriptive Poetiken Emotionalisierender Figurendarstellung, in: Claudia Hillebrandt/Elisabeth Kampmann (eds.), Sympathie und Literatur. Zur Relevanz des Sympathiekonzeptes für die Literaturwissenschaft, Berlin 2014, 153–167.

Bareis, J. Alexander, ›Empathie ist immer gut‹. Literatur, Emotion und imaginative resistance am Beispiel von Vladimir Nabokovs Lolita, in: Claudia Hillebrandt/Elisabeth Kampmann (eds.), Sympathie und Literatur. Zur Relevanz des Sympathiekonzeptes für die Literaturwissenschaft, Berlin 2014, 128–152.

Barsalou, Lawrence W., Grounded Cognition, Annual Review of Psychology 59:1 (2008), 617–645. [Crossref] [Web of Science]

Batson, C. Daniel et al., Similarity and Nurturance. Two Possible Sources of Empathy for Strangers, Basic and Applied Social Psychology 27:1 (2005), 15–25.

Bernhardt, Boris C./Tania Singer, The Neural Basis of Empathy, Annual Review of Neuroscience 35 (2012), 1–23. [Crossref]

Collins, Suzanne, The Hunger Games [2008], London 2011.

Colman, Andrew M., A Dictionary of Psychology, Online Edition, Oxford 2015.

Feldman Barrett, Lisa, Psychological Construction. The Darwinian Approach to the Science of Emotion, Emotion Review 5:4 (2013), 379–389. [Crossref]

Fong, Katrina/Justin B. Mullin/Raymond A. Mar, What You Read Matters. The Role of Fiction Genre in Predicting Interpersonal Sensitivity, Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 7:4 (2013), 370–376.

Green, Melanie/Timothy C. Brock, The Role of Transportation in the Persuasiveness of Public Narratives, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79:5 (2000), 701–721.

Green, Melanie/Timothy C. Brock/Jeffrey Strange (eds.), Narrative Impact. Social and Cognitive Foundations, Mahwah, NJ 2002.

Gymnich, Marion/Ansgar Nünning, Funktionsgeschichtliche Ansätze. Terminologische Grundlagen und Funktionsbestimmungen von Literatur, in: M.G./A.N. (eds.), Funktionen von Literatur. Theoretische Grundlagen und Modellinterpretationen, Trier 2005, 3–28.

Hillebrandt, Claudia/Elisabeth Kampmann, Sympathie und Literatur. Einführende Bemerkungen zu einem vernachlässigten Verhältnis, in: C.H./E.K. (eds.), Sympathie und Literatur. Zur Relevanz des Sympathiekonzeptes für die Literaturwissenschaft, Berlin 2014, 7–32.

Iser, Wolfgang, Der Akt des Lesens. Theorie ästhetischer Wirkung, München 1976.

Kaul, Susanne, Wie der Leser mit dem Helden warm Wird. Zu Schillers Verbrecher aus verlorener Ehre, in: Claudia Hillebrandt/Elisabeth Kampmann (eds.), Sympathie und Literatur. Zur Relevanz des Sympathiekonzeptes für die Literaturwissenschaft, Berlin 2014, 236–250.

Keen, Suzanne, A Theory of Narrative Empathy, Narrative 14:3 (2007), 207–236. [Crossref]

Kidd, David Comer/Emanuele Castano, Reading Literary Fiction Improves Theory of Mind, Science 342:6156 (2013), 377–380.

Kotovych, Maria et al., Textual Determinants of a Component of Literary Identification, Scientific Study of Literature 1:2 (2011), 260–291.

Lehnen, Christine, Defining Dystopia. A Genre Between The Circle and The Hunger Games. A Functional Approach to Fiction, Marburg 2015.

Mar, Raymond A. et al., Emotion and Narrative Fiction. Interactive Influences Before, During, and After Reading, Cognition and Emotion 25:5 (2011), 818–833. [Crossref]

Miall, David S./Don Kuiken, What is Literariness? Three Components of Literary Reading, Discourse Processes 28:2 (1999), 121–138. [Crossref]

Mosendz, Polly, Amazon Has Basically No Competition Among Online-Booksellers, The Wired, 2014, http://www.thewire.com/business/2014/05/amazon-has-basically-no-competition-among-online-booksellers/371917/ (03.03.2015).

Noddings, Nel, Caring. A Feminine Approach to Ethics & Moral Education [1984], Berkeley, CA 22003.

Nook, Eric C./Kristen A. Lindquist/Jamil Zaki, A New Look at Emotion Perception. Concept Speed and Shape Facial Emotion Recognition, Emotion 15:5 (2015), 569–578. [Crossref]

Oatley, Keith, Emotions and the Story Worlds of Fiction, in: Melanie Green/Jeffrey Strange/Timothy C. Brock (eds.), Narrative Impact. Social and Cognitive Foundations, Mahwah, NJ 2002, 39–69.

Oatley, Keith, From Moral Sentiments to fMRI. Empathic Emotions of Social Relating and of Engaging with Fiction, https://sites.google.com/site/onfiction/OatleyEmpathy‌Talk.pptx?attredirects=0 (21.06.16).

Oatley, Keith/Raymond A. Mar, The Function of Fiction is the Abstraction and Simulation of Social Experience, Perspectives on Psychological Sciences 3:3 (2008), 173–192.

Schank, Roger C./Tamara R. Berman, The Pervasive Role of Stories in Knowledge and Action, in: Melanie Green/Jeffrey Strange/Timothy C. Brock (eds.), Narrative Impact. Social and Cognitive Foundations, Mahwah, NJ 2002, 287–313.

Scheele, Brigitte, Empathie und Sympathie bei der Literatur-Rezeption. Ein Henne-Ei-Problem?, in: Claudia Hillebrandt/Elisabeth Kampmann (eds.),Sympathie und Literatur. Zur Relevanz des Sympathiekonzeptes für die Literaturwissenschaft, Berlin 2014, 35–48.

Siemer, Matthias/Iris Mauss/James J. Gross, Same Situation-Different Emotions. How Appraisals Shape Our Emotions, Emotion 7:3 (2007), 592–600. [Crossref]

Sklar, Howard, The Art of Sympathy in Fiction. Forms of Ethical and Emotional Persuasion, Amsterdam 2013.

Smith, Craig A./Phoebe C. Ellsworth, Patterns of Cognitive Appraisal in Emotion, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 48:4 (1985), 813–838.

Sommer, Roy, Funktionsgeschichten. Überlegungen zur Verwendung des Funktionsbegriffs in der Literaturwissenschaft und Anregungen zu seiner terminologischen Differenzierung, Literaturwissenschaftliches Jahrbuch 41 (2000), 319–341.

Wegmann, Thomas, Warentest und Selbstmanagement. Literaturkritik im Web 2.0 als Teil nachbürgerlichen Wissens- und Beurteilungskulturen, in: Matthias Beilein/Claudia Stockinger/Simone Winko (eds.), Kanon, Wertung und Vermittlung. Literatur in der Wissensgesellschaft, Berlin/Boston 2011, 279–292.

Winko, Simone/Katharina Prinz, Sympathielenkung und textinterne Wertungen. Überlegungen zu ihrer Untersuchung und exemplarische Analyse der Figur des ›unglücklichen Mordgehilfen‹ Olivier Brusson, in: Claudia Hillebrandt/Elisabeth Kampmann (eds.), Sympathie und Literatur. Zur Relevanz des Sympathiekonzeptes für die Literaturwissenschaft, Berlin 2014, 99–127.

Wondra, Joshua D./Phoebe C. Ellsworth, An Appraisal Theory of Empathy and Other Vicarious Emotional Experiences, Psychological Review 122:3 (2015), 411–428. [Web of Science] [Crossref]

Zaki, Jamil, Empathy. A Motivated Account, Psychological Bulletin 140:6 (2014), 1608–1647. [Web of Science] [Crossref]

Zaki, Jamil, Empathy Is a Moral Force, http://ssnl.stanford.edu/sites/default/files/pdf/‌zaki_forthcoming_empMorality.pdf?width=85%&height=85%&iframe=true (15.05.2016).

Zaki, Jamil et al., The Anatomy of Suffering. Understanding the Relationship Between Nociceptive and Empathic Pain, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20:4 (2016), 249–259.

Zaki, Jamil/Kevin Ochsner, Empathy, in: Lisa Feldman Barret/Michael Lewis/Jeannette M. Haviland-Jones (eds.), Handbook of Emotions [1993], New York 42016, 871–884.

2017-07-01

JLTonline ISSN 1862-8990

Copyright © by the author. All rights reserved.
This work may be copied for non-profit educational use if proper credit is given to the author and JLTonline.
For other permission, please contact JLTonline.

How to cite this item:

Abstract of: Christine Lehnen, Exploring Narratives’ Powers of Emotional Persuasion through Character Involvement: A Working Heuristic.

In: JLTonline (07.01.2017)

URL: http://www.jltonline.de/index.php/articles/article/view/866/1939

A Persistent Identifier can be found in the PDF-Version of this article.